
By LINDA GORDON
Published: March 25, 2010
Nell
Irvin Painter’s title, “The History of White People,” is a provocation
in several ways: it’s monumental in sweep, and it’s absurd grandiosity should
call to mind the fact that writing a “History of Black People” might seem
perfectly reasonable to white people. But the title is literally accurate,
because the book traces characterizations of the lighter-skinned people we call
white today, starting with the ancient Scythians. For those who have not yet
registered how much these characterizations have changed, let me assure you that
sensory observation was not the basis of racial nomenclature.
Some ancient
descriptions did note color, as when the ancient Greeks recognized that their
“barbaric” northern neighbors, Scythians and Celts, had lighter skin than
Greeks considered normal. Most ancient peoples defined population differences
culturally, not physically, and often regarded lighter people as less
civilized. Centuries later, European travel writers regarded the light-skinned
Circassians, a k a Caucasians, as people best fit only for slavery, yet at the
same time labeled Circassian slave women the epitome of beauty. Exoticizing and
sexualizing women of
allegedly inferior “races” has a long and continuous
history in racial thought; it’s just that today they are usually darker-skinned
women.
“Whiteness
studies” have so proliferated in the last two decades that historians might be
forgiven a yawn in response to being told that racial divisions are
fundamentally arbitrary, and that deciding who is white has been not only fluid
but also heavily influenced by class and culture. In some Latin American
countries, for example, the term blanquearse, to bleach oneself, is used to mean
moving upward in class status. But this concept — the social and cultural
construction of race over time — remains harder for many people to understand
than, say, the notion that gender is a social and cultural construction, unlike
sex. As recently as 10 years ago, some of my undergraduate students at the University of Wisconsin heard my explanations of critical race
theory as a denial of observable physical differences.
I wish I had
had this book to offer them. Painter, a renowned historian recently retired
from Princeton, has written an unusual study: an intellectual history, with
occasional excursions to examine vernacular usage, for popular audiences. It
has much to teach everyone, including whiteness experts, but it is accessible
and breezy, its coverage broad and therefore necessarily superficial.
The modern
intellectual history of whiteness began among the 18th-century German scholars
who invented racial “science.” Johann Joachim Winckelmann made the ancient
Greeks his models of beauty by imagining them white-skinned; he may even have
suppressed his own (correct) suspicion that their statues, though copied by the
Romans in white marble, had originally been painted. The Dutchman Petrus Camper
calculated the proportions and angles of the ideal face and skull, and produced
a scale that awarded a perfect rating to the head of a Greek god and ranked
Europeans as the runners-up, earning 80 out of 100.
The Englishman Charles
White collected skulls that he arranged from lowest to highest degree of
perfection. He did not think he was seeing the gradual improvement of the human
species, but assumed rather the polygenesis theory: the different races arose
from separate divine creations and were designed with a range of quality.
The modern concept of a Caucasian race, which students my age were
taught in school, came from Johann Friedrich Blumenbach of Göttingen, the most
influential of this generation of race scholars. Switching from skulls to skin,
he divided humans into five races by color — white, yellow, copper, tawny, and
tawny-black to jet-black — but he ascribed these differences to climate. Still
convinced that people of the Caucasus were the paragons of beauty, he placed
residents of North Africa and India in the Caucasian category, sliding into a
linguistic analysis based on the common derivation of Indo-European languages.
That category, Painter notes, soon slipped free of any geographic or linguistic
moorings and became a quasi-scientific term for a race known as “white.”
Some great
American heroes, notably Thomas Jefferson and Ralph Waldo Emerson, absorbed Blumenbach’s
influence but relabeled the categories of white superiority. They adopted the
Saxons as their ideal, imagining Americans as direct and unalloyed descendants
of the English, later including the Germans. In general, Western labels for
racial superiority moved thus: Caucasian → Saxon → Teutonic → Nordic → Aryan → white/Anglo.
The spread of
evolutionary theory required a series of theoretical shifts, to cope with
changing understandings of what is heritable. When hereditary thought produced
eugenics, the effort to breed superior human beings, it relied mostly on
inaccurate genetics. Nevertheless, eugenic “science” became authoritative from
the late 19th century through the 1930s. Eugenics gave rise to laws in at least
30 states authorizing forced sterilization of the ostensibly feeble-minded and
the hereditarily criminal. Painter cites an estimate of 65,000 sterilized
against their will by 1968, after which a combined feminist and civil rights
campaign succeeded in radically restricting forced sterilization. While blacks and American Indians were
disproportionately victimized, intelligence testing added many immigrants and
others of “inferior stock,” predominantly Appalachian whites, to the rolls of
the surgically sterilized.
In the long
run, the project of measuring “intelligence” probably did more than eugenics to
stigmatize and hold back the nonwhite. Researchers gave I.Q. tests to 1,750,000
recruits in World War I and found that the average mental age, for those 18 and
over, was 13.08 years. That experiment in mass testing failed owing to the
Army’s insistence that even the lowest ranked usually became model soldiers.
But I.Q. testing achieved success in driving the anti-immigration movement.
The tests allowed
calibrated rankings of Americans of different ancestries — the English at the
top, Poles on the bottom. Returning to head measurements, other researchers
computed with new categories the proportion of different “blood” in people of
different races: Belgians were 60 percent Nordic (the superior European race)
and 40 percent Alpine, while the Irish were 30 percent Nordic and 70 percent
Mediterranean (the inferior European race). Sometimes politics produced
immediate changes in these supposedly objective findings: World War I caused
the downgrading of Germans from heavily Nordic to heavily Alpine.
Painter points
out, but without adequate discussion, that the adoration of whiteness became
particularly problematic for women, as pale blue-eyed blondes became, like so
many unattainable desires, a reminder of what was second-class about the rest
of us. Among the painfully comic absurdities that racial science produced was
the “beauty map” constructed by Francis Galton around the turn of the 20th
century: he classified people as good, medium or bad; he categorized those he
saw by using pushpins and thus demonstrated that London ranked highest and
Aberdeen lowest in average beauty.
Rankings of
intelligence and beauty supported escalating anti-Catholicism and anti-Semitism
in early-20th-century America. Both prejudices racialized non-Protestant
groups. But Painter misses some crucial regional differences. While Jews and
Italians were nonwhite in the East, they had long been white in San Francisco,
where the racial “inferiors” were the Chinese. Although the United States
census categorized Mexican-Americans as white through 1930, census enumerators
in the Southwest, working from a different racial understanding, ignored those
instructions and marked them “M” for Mexican.
In the same
period, anarchist or socialist beliefs became a sign of racial inferiority, a
premise strengthened by the presence of many immigrants and Jews among
early-20th-century radicals. Whiteness thus became a method of stigmatizing
dissenting ideas, a marker of ideological respectability; Painter should have
investigated this phenomenon further. Also missing from the book is an analysis
of the all-important question: Who benefits and how from the imprimatur of
whiteness? Political elites and employers of low-wage labor, to choose just two
groups, actively policed the boundaries of whiteness.
But I cannot
fault Nell Painter’s choices — omissions to keep a book widely readable. Often,
scholarly interpretation is transmitted through textbooks that oversimplify and
even bore their readers with vague generalities. Far better for a large audience
to learn about whiteness from a distinguished scholar in an insightful and
lively exposition.
Linda Gordon is a professor of history at
New York University and the author, most recently, of “Dorothea Lange: A Life
Beyond Limits.”
Source: NYTimes
No comments:
Post a Comment